Sensory Systems/Octopus
Octopus: Sensorimotor System
editIntroduction
editOne of the most interesting non-primate is the octopus. The most interesting feature about this non-primate is its arm movement. In these invertebrates, the control of the arm is especially complex because the arm can be moved in any direction, with a virtually infinite number of degrees of freedom. In the octopus, the brain only has to send a command to the arm to do the action—the entire recipe of how to do it is embedded in the arm itself. Observations indicate that octopuses reduce the complexity of controlling their arms by keeping their arm movements to set, stereotypical patterns. To find out if octopus arms have minds of their own, the researchers cut off the nerves in an octopus arm from the other nerves in its body, including the brain. They then tickled and stimulated the skin on the arm. The arm behaved in an identical fashion to what it would in a healthy octopus. The implication is that the brain only has to send a single move command to the arm, and the arm will do the rest.
In this chapter we discuss in detail the sensory system of an octopus and focus on the sensory motor system in this non-primate.
Octopus - The intelligent non-primate
editOctopuses have two eyes and four pairs of arms, and they are bilaterally symmetric. An octopus has a hard beak, with its mouth at the center point of the arms. Octopuses have no internal or external skeleton (although some species have a vestigial remnant of a shell inside their mantle), allowing them to squeeze through tight places. Octopuses are among the most intelligent and behaviorally flexible of all invertebrates.
The most interesting feature of the octopuses is their arm movements. For goal directed arm movements, the nervous system in octopus generates a sequence of motor commands that brings the arm towards the target. Control of the arm is especially complex because the arm can be moved in any direction, with a virtually infinite number of degrees of freedom. The basic motor program for voluntary movement is embedded within the neural circuitry of the arm itself.[1]
Arm Movements in Octopus
editIn the hierarchical organization in octopus, the brain only has to send a command to the arm to do the action. The entire recipe of how to do it is embedded in the arm itself. By the use of the arms octopus walks, seizes its pray, or rejects unwanted objects and also obtains a wide range of mechanical and chemical information about its immediate environment.
Octopus arms, unlike human arms, are not limited in their range of motion by elbow, wrist, and shoulder joints. To accomplish goals such as reaching for a meal or swimming, however, an octopus must be able to control its eight appendages. The octopus arm can move in any direction using virtually infinite degrees of freedom. This ability results from the densely packed flexible muscle fibers along the arm of the octopus.
Observations indicate that octopuses reduce the complexity of controlling their arms by keeping their arm movements to set, stereotypical patterns.[2] For example, the reaching movement always consists of a bend that propagates along the arm toward the tip. Since octopuses always use the same kind of movement to extend their arms, the commands that generate the pattern are stored in the arm itself, not in the central brain. Such a mechanism further reduces the complexity of controlling a flexible arm. These flexible arms are controlled by an elaborate peripheral nervous system containing 5 × 107 neurons distributed along each arm. 4 × 105 of these are motor neurons, which innervate the intrinsic muscles of the arm and locally control muscle action.
Whenever it is required, the nervous system in octopus generates a sequence of motor commands which in turn produces forces and corresponding velocities making the limb reach the target. The movements are simplified by the use of optimal trajectories made through vectorial summation and superposition of basic movements. This requires that the muscles are quite flexible.
Central nervous system
editUnlike other invertebrates, octopuses and their relatives (cuttlefish and squid) have evolved larger nervous systems and more sophisticated cognitive abilities. Although octopus brains differ in anatomy from vertebrate brains, they still share a variety of similar features, including patterns of short - and long-term memory, patterns of sleep, and the ability to recognize individuals and explore objects[3]. Octopuses can navigate simple mazes and use visual cues to distinguish between two familiar environments. More interestingly, octopuses can even use their tentacles to unscrew jars to get to the food inside.
Octopuses, cuttlefish and squid all belong to the class Cephalopod, which also includes extinct creatures such as ammonoids and arrowstones. As cephalopods evolved, their shells all internalized or disappeared altogether. At the same time, another shift took place: some cephalopods experienced a marked increase in intelligence. Although it is difficult to quantify the extent of an animal's intellectual development, these animals have evolved large nervous systems, including brains, at least in appearance. The common octopus has about 500 million neurons in its body, which is in the same level as many mammals and close to that of dogs[4]. And this number is obviously much larger than that of all other invertebrates.
The remarkable intelligence of octopuses can be vividly illustrated by a short story. An octopus trapped in a water tank appears to be watching in a subtle way to see if humans notice it, and when they don't, it moves and tries to escape the tank, said David Scheel, a biologist from Alaska Pacific University. Even when then human is wearing a mask, the octopus can still recognize when a human is there[4].
The anatomy of an octopus brain is very different from that of a vertebrate brain. Most of an octopus's neurons are not in its brain, but in its arms. Functionally, the octopus brain is not responsible for processing most sensory information and motor commands. Instead, the brain takes in only part of the sensory information and sends out simple commands. These instructions are not materialized until they are sent to the various organs, which means that the octopus's brain is quite independent of what it sells. However, this does not mean that octopus brains are not important in behavioral decision-making. Compare this to the hierarchical structure of the vertebrate nervous system (for example, the central nervous system integrates and responds to input from the peripheral nervous system). More accurately, the relationship between the octopus's brain and arms can be described as "collaborative". In 2011, researchers Tamar Gutnick and Ruth Byrne, along with Hochner and Kuba, conducted an experiment to examine whether octopuses could learn to guide an individual arm to specific locations in a maze in order to obtain food[5]. In this experiment, the chemical sensors on the octopus' arms were not enough to guide them to food, and the arms had to leave the water at some point to reach their target. But the walls of the maze are set to transparent, so the octopus can see the food target visually. In this way, the octopus must visually guide its arm through the maze to reach the food target. Although it took the octopuses a long time to learn how to do this, all the animals in the experiment eventually succeeded. This suggests that octopus vision can guide the arms to perform certain movements. Also, the paper notes that when the octopus is on a food-finding mission, its arms seem to move toward the target while exploring locally. As a result, there appear to be two forms of control working together: central control of the arm's overall path guided through vision, and fine-tuning of the arm's exploration of the surrounding space in conjunction with the arm itself.
The Nervous System of the Arms
editThe eight arms of the octopus are elongated, tapering, muscular organs, projecting from the head and regularly arranged around the mouth. The inner surface of each arm bears a double row of suckers, each sucker alternating with that of the opposite row. There are about 300 suckers on each arm.[6]
The arms perform both motor and sensory functions. The nervous system in the arms of the octopus is represented by the nerve ganglia, subserving motor and inter-connecting functions. The peripheral nerve cells represent the sensory systems. There exists a close functional relationship between the nerve ganglia and the peripheral nerve cells.
General anatomy of the arm
editThe muscles of the arm can be divided into three separate groups, each having a certain degree of anatomical and functional independence:
- Intrinsic muscles of the arm,
- Intrinsic muscles of the suckers, and
- Acetabulo-brachial muscles (connects the suckers to the arm muscles).
Each of these three groups of muscles comprises three muscle bundles at right angles to one another. Each bundle is innervated separately from the surrounding units and shows a remarkable autonomy.In spite of the absence of a bony or cartilaginous skeleton, octopus can produce arm movements using the contraction and relaxation of different muscles. Behaviorally, the longitudinal muscles shorten the arm and play major role in seizing objects carrying them to mouth, and the oblique and transverse muscles lengthen the arms and are used by octopus for rejecting unwanted objects.
Six main nerve centers lie in the arm and are responsible for the performance of these sets of muscles. The axial nerve cord is by far the most important motor and integrative center of the arm. The eight cords one in each arm contains altogether 3.5 × 108 neurons. Each axial cord is linked by means of connective nerve bundles with five sets of more peripheral nerve centers, the four intramuscular nerve cords, lying among the intrinsic muscles of the arm, and the ganglia of the suckers, situated in the peduncle just beneath the acetabular cup of each sucker.
All these small peripheral nerves contain motor neurons and receive sensory fibers from deep muscle receptors which play the role of local reflex centers. The motor innervation of the muscles of the arm is thus provided not only by the motor neurons of the axial nerve cord, which receives pre-ganglionic fibers from the brain, but also by these more peripheral motor centers.
Sensory Nervous system
editThe arms contain a complex and extensive sensory system. Deep receptors in the three main muscle systems of the arms, provide the animal with a widespread sensory apparatus for collecting information from muscles. Many primary receptors lie in the epithelium covering the surface of the arm. The sucker, and particularly its rim, has the greatest number of these sensory cells, while the skin of the arm is rather less sensitive. Several tens of thousands of receptors lie in each sucker.
Three main morphological types of receptors are found in arms of an octopus. These are round cells, irregular multipolar cells, and tapered ciliated cells. All these elements send their processes centripetally towards the ganglia. The functional significance of these three types of receptors is still not very well known and can only be conjectured. It has been suggested that the round and multipolar receptors may record mechanical stimuli, while ciliated receptors are likely to be chemo-receptors.
The ciliated receptors do not send their axons directly to the ganglia but the axons meet encapsulated neurons lying underneath the epithelium and make synaptic contacts with the dendritic processes of these. This linkage helps in reduction of input between primary nerve cells. Round and multipolar receptors on the other hand send their axons directly to the ganglia where the motor neurons lie.
Functioning of peripheral nervous system in arm movements
editBehavioral experiments suggest that information regarding the movement of the muscles does not reach the learning centers of the brain, and morphological observations prove that the deep receptors send their axons to peripheral centers such as the ganglion of the sucker or the intramuscular nerve cords.[7] The information regarding the stretch or movement of the muscles is used in local reflexes only.
When the dorsal part of the axial nerve cord that contains the axonal tracts from the brain is stimulated by electrical signals, movements in entire arm are still noticed. The movements are triggered by the stimulation which is provided and is not directly driven by the stimuli coming from the brain. Thus, arm extensions are evoked by stimulation of the dorsal part of the axial nerve cord. In contrast, the stimulation of the muscles within the same area or the ganglionic part of the cord evokes only local muscular contractions. The implication is that the brain only has to send a single move command to the arm, and the arm will do the rest.
A dorsally oriented bend propagates along the arm causing the suckers to point in the direction of the movement. As the bend propagates, the part of the arm proximal to the bend remains extended. For further conformations that an octopus arm has a mind of its own, the nerves in an octopus arm have been cut off from the other nerves in its body, including the brain. Movements resembling normal arm extensions were initiated in amputated arms by electrical stimulation of the nerve cord or by tactile stimulation of the skin or suckers.
It has been noted that the bend propagations are more readily initiated when a bend is created manually before stimulation. If the fully relaxed arm is stimulated, the initial movement is triggered by the stimuli, which follows the same bend propagation. The nervous system of the arm thus, not only drives local reflexes but controls complex movements involving the entire arm.
These evoked movements are almost kinematically identical to the movements of freely behaving octopus. When stimulated, a severed arm shows an active propagation of the muscle activity as in natural arm extensions. Movements evoked from similar initial arm postures result in similar paths, while different starting postures result in different final paths.
As the extensions evoked in denervated octopus arms are qualitatively and kinematically similar to natural arm extensions, an underlying motor program seems to be controlling the movements which are embedded in the neuromuscular system of the arm, which does not require central control.
- ↑ G. S. et al., Control of Octopus Arm Extension by a Peripheral Motor Program . Science 293, 1845, 2001.
- ↑ Y. Gutfreund, Organization of octopus arm movements: a model system for study- ing the control of flexible arms. Journal of Neuroscience 16, 7297, 1996.
- ↑ Frank, Marcos G., et al. "A preliminary analysis of sleep-like states in the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis." PLoS One 7.6 (2012): e38125.
- ↑ a b P. Godfrey-Smith, The Mind of an Octopus, Sci Am Mind, vol. 28, no. 1, pp. 62–69, Dec. 2016
- ↑ Gutnick, Tamar et al. “Octopus vulgaris uses visual information to determine the location of its arm.” Current biology : CB vol. 21,6 (2011): 460-2. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2011.01.052
- ↑ P. Graziadei, The anatomy of the nervous system of Octopus vulgaris, J. Z. Young. Clarendon, Oxford, 1971.
- ↑ M. J. Wells, The orientation of octopus. Ergeb. Biol. 26, 40-54, 1963.